Dsa Wege Des Schwartz Pdf

Posted : admin On 09.10.2019
  1. Dsa Wege Des Schwartz Pdf Editor
  2. Dsa Wege Des Schwartz Pdf Download
  3. Dsa Wege Des Schwartz Pdf Converter
Dsa Wege Des Schwartz Pdf

Jun 22, 2019  Dsa Wege Des Schwartz Pdf. Don’t worry about it, lol. Milenko de currently reading it Apr 04, Het oog des meesters ‘The eye of the master’. Four hundred volunteers underwent clinical and radiological evaluations between March.

Measurements and Results: Bidirectional relationships between, on the one hand, workplace demands, decision authority, and support, and, on the other hand, sleep disturbances (reflecting lack of sleep continuity) and awakening problems (reflecting feelings of being insufficiently restored), were investigated by structural equation modeling. All factors were modeled as latent variables and adjusted for gender, age, marital status, education, alcohol consumption, and job change.

Concerning sleep disturbances, the best fitting models were the “forward” causal model for demands and the “reverse” causal model for support. Regarding awakening problems, reciprocal models fitted the data best. Conclusions: Cross-lagged analyses indicates a weak relationship between demands at Time 1 and sleep disturbances at Time 2, a “reverse” relationship from support T1 to sleep disturbances T2, and bidirectional associations between work characteristics and awakening problems. In contrast to an earlier study on demands, control, sleep quality, and fatigue, this study suggests reverse and reciprocal in addition to the commonly hypothesized causal relationships between work characteristics and sleep problems based on a 2-year time lag. INTRODUCTION About one-third of all adults in Europe have complaints of sleep problems, such as difficulties falling asleep, maintaining sleep, early morning awakenings, and non-restorative sleep several times a week. And as much as 9% of the population fulfill the criteria for an insomnia diagnosis, with complaints lasting for at least a month, accompanied by impairment in social, occupational, or other areas of functioning. A core symptom of insomnia is non-restorative sleep, a subjective feeling at awakening of having had an unrestful sleep or not being refreshed upon awakening.

A considerable part of the population experiences non-restorative or unrefreshing sleep which is not due to lack of sleep. Non-restorative sleep can thus occur independently of other insomnia symptoms. Although sleep problems may be caused by health deficits, they might also precede health problems like poor mental health, heart disease, obesity, diabetes, and increase mortality risk. Chronic stress associated with hyperactivity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenocortical (HPA) and sympatho-adreno-medullary (SAM) systems might lead to impaired sleep.

A number of working conditions that may constitute stressors such as long work hours, shift work, high workload/demands/job strain, poor social support, effort-reward imbalance, organizational injustice, and workplace bullying have been linked to sleep problems; – whereas retirement has been shown to be followed by a substantial decrease in sleep disturbances. In a survey in the US, the most frequent self-reported cause of sleeping difficulties was indeed work-related stress, and an increase of work-related sleep problems has been demonstrated among the Swedish working population during the 1990s. Some recent investigations using the demand-control and effort-reward imbalance models show prospective relationships between work characteristics and sleep disturbances and/or insomnia., Thus far, however, evidence from longitudinal studies is limited, and more evidence is needed to elucidate causality and the directions of the relationships. De Lange et al. Recently investigated associations between job demands and control on the one hand, and sleep quality and fatigue on the other hand, and found no reverse or reciprocal patterns. However, their sample consisted of a relatively young and predominantly male population.

Furthermore, with fewer measurements on sleep quality and fatigue, the chance of detecting reverse or reciprocal associations was lower than of relationships in the hypothesized direction. In the present study we investigate the longitudinal relationships between the components of the demand-control-support model and sleep problems (sleep disturbances and awakening problems). The study examines both the effects of work characteristics on later sleep problems (subsequently referred to as “forward” causation, i.e., in the direction most often assumed in the literature), and the relationship between sleep problems and work characteristics (referred to as “reverse” causation) two years later. Study Participants The study was based on the Swedish Longitudinal Occupational Survey of Health (SLOSH). In SLOSH, participants in the Swedish Work Environment Survey (SWES) from 2003 are followed up by postal questionnaires every second year.

SWES is in turn a subsample of respondents to the Swedish Labor Force Survey (LFS), including gainfully employed men and women from different labor market sectors and occupations. Accordingly, SLOSH is approximately representative of the Swedish working population in 2003. The first follow-up (Wave 1) of SWES 2003 was conducted in 2006, with 5985 (65%) respondents: 5141 “gainfully employed” and 844 “not gainfully employed.” Respondents were categorized as gainfully employed if they had responded to the questionnaire for those who had worked on average ≥ 30% during the past 3 months. In 2008 (Wave 2), participants in SWES 2003 were again followed up, with a total of 5557 (61%) respondents: 4672 gainfully employed and 885 not gainfully employed. The present study included those individuals who took part in the 2006 as well as the 2008 follow-up survey (n = 4690; 78% of all participants in Wave 1), and who were categorized as gainfully employed at both occasions (n = 3644; see ). Subjects with incomplete data on any of the questions of interest were excluded, resulting in a study sample of 3041 individuals. Compared to all participants in SWES 2003 (n = 9214), a higher proportion of the respondents included in the final study sample were in the age range 40-59 in 2003, and a higher proportion had university education.

However, the gender distribution was virtually the same, as were the mean scores on demand, control, and support proxy measures from SWES 2003, and the mean scores on items measuring if respondents get enough sleep, have a hard time sleeping because of thoughts of work, and feel tired and listless after work., The study was based on informed consent and has been approved by the Regional Research Ethics Board in Stockholm. Measures The questionnaires for gainfully employed in Wave 1 and 2 included numerous questions about the psychosocial work environment, work organization, health, and health-related complaints. Demands, control and support at work were measured by the Swedish version of the Demand-Control Questionnaire (DCQ).

Demands at work were assessed by 5 questions (working fast, working intensively, too much effort, enough time, and conflicting demands) with 5 response options (1 = Never/almost never – 4 = Often). Cronbach α was 0.75 at Time 1 and 0.72 at Time 2. Decision authority was assessed with 2 questions (choice in how you do your work and what you do) with 4 response options (1 = To a very small extent/not at all – 4 = To a very great extent).

Tape Deck Heart. By Frank Turner. Publication date 2013-04-23. Language English. See also MusicBrainz (release) [MusicBrainz (release)]; MusicBrainz (artist) [MusicBrainz (artist)]. Identifier mbid-979c60da-6713-4ab4-b7ae-1f9829104eb0. Check out Tape Deck Heart [Explicit] by Frank Turner on Amazon Music. Stream ad-free or purchase CD's and MP3s now on Amazon.com. Tape deck player. Tape Deck Heart. By Frank Turner. Publication date 2013-04-22. Language English. See also MusicBrainz (release) [MusicBrainz (release)]; MusicBrainz (artist) [MusicBrainz (artist)]; Amazon [Amazon]. Identifier mbid-83de4db0-b124-457c-8faf-56e663f6fdd1.

Cronbach α was 0.77 at Time 1 and 0.73 at Time 2. Social support was measured with 6 questions (calm and pleasant atmosphere, good spirit of unity, colleagues are there for me, people understand a bad day, get on well with my colleagues, get on well with my superiors) with 4 response options (1 = Strongly disagree – 4 = Strongly agree). Cronbach α was 0.85 at both time points. A question about working hours was modified from SWES and categorized into 1 = day work; 2 = evening, shift work (2 shifts), discretionary/unregulated working hours or other; and 3 = night work and shift work (3 shifts). Physical strain was assessed by another question from SWES: Does your work sometimes involve physical labor, that is, do you physically exert yourself more than one does when walking and standing and moving around in a normal way?

(response options 1 = No, not at all – 6 = Nearly all the time). Sleep disturbances (reflecting lack of sleep continuity) were assessed with 4 questions (difficulty falling asleep, repeated awakenings, early awakening, disturbed sleep); whereas awakening problems (reflecting feelings of being insufficiently restored) were measured with 3 questions (difficulty awakening, not well-rested, exhausted at awakening), all from the Karolinska Sleep Questionnaire., The following response options were used: 1 = Never – 6 = Always/5 times a week or more. Cronbach α was 0.85 for disturbed sleep and 0.80 for awakening problems at both time points. Information about gender and age was retrieved from baseline register data. The questionnaires further included information about marital status (1 = single, 2 = married or cohabiting), small children (0-5 years of age) at home (1 = yes, 2 = no), educational level (1 = compulsory, 2 = 2-year upper secondary/vocational training, 3 = 3 or 4-year upper secondary education, 4 = university or equivalent. Analytic Strategy The data were first analyzed by means of the Predictive Analytics Software (PASW) Statistics 18.0. A missing value analysis was performed to assess the pattern of missing data.

As Little's missing completely at random (MCAR) test was nonsignificant, indicating that data were missing completely at random, deletion of individuals with missing data was presumed to result in a random subsample of the original target sample. The distribution of the items measuring work characteristics and sleep problems was inspected and found approximately symmetric for the majority of the items. The sleep items exhibited some skewness, but since most of them were only moderately skewed we did not use any transformation. The bivariate relationships between component scores at Time 1 were also checked and were approximately linear.

Structural equation models were then fitted using the LISREL 8.8 Software. Estimations were based on covariance matrices using the robust maximum likelihood method. We first tested the measurement model for sleep problems. A confirmatory factor analysis based on data from Time point 1 supported further analyses of sleep problems by the 2 indices: sleep disturbances and awakening difficulties. Compared to a null model with 7 distinct factors, a model with all sleep items as one factor, a model with 2 orthogonal factors (sleep disturbances and awakening problems), the 2-factor oblique solution provided significantly (P 0.90 indicate a good fit to the data. Finally a smaller value on the Akaike Information Criterion (AIC) was used to indicate a more parsimonious model. Local fit was also considered for structural paths in terms of standardized beta regression coefficients (β) and t-values.

Moreover, the χ 2 difference test was used to determine which of the competing models that provided the best fit to the data. Models 1-3 were all compared to the null model. If Models 1 or 2 as well as Model 3 showed significantly better fit than the null model, Model 3 was subsequently compared to the better-fitting of Models 1 and 2. Sleep Disturbances In fit statistics and model comparisons are presented for the competing models of the demand-control-support components and sleep disturbances. According to the RMSEA, GFI, and CFI fit statistics, all models concerning sleep disturbances provided a good or very good fit to the data. However, the χ 2 comparison test of the competing models including demands and sleep disturbances showed that the fit of Model 1, including forward paths, and Model 3, including reciprocal paths, were significantly better than the fit of the null model. A comparison of the former 2 models further showed that Model 3 did not provide significantly better fit to the data than Model 1.

Consequently, the models including the forward path from Time 1 demands to Time 2 sleep disturbances provided the best fit to the data. The competing models for decision authority and sleep disturbances did not differ significantly from the null model, indicating that the model with only auto-regressions best described the relationships between the variables. Model 2 (with reverse paths) and 3 (with reciprocal paths) including support differed significantly from Model 0. However, Model 3 did not differ significantly from Model 2, indicating that the models, including the reverse causal path from Time 1 sleep disturbances to Time 2 support, provided the best fit to the data. Model fit and comparison for structural equation models of work characteristics in relation to sleep disturbances The regression coefficients for the forward and reverse structural paths for models concerning work stress and sleep disturbances are presented in. The path from T1 demands to T2 sleep disturbances was borderline significant, but not the path from T1 sleep disturbances to T2 demands.

T1 sleep disturbances, on the other hand, showed a significant positive β coefficient in relation to T2 support, whereas the effect of support on subsequent sleep disturbances did not reach statistical significance. Factor loadings can be found in the. Graphs of the full basic LISREL models are also in the supplement (–). The regression coefficients were virtually unaltered and the results of the model comparisons were the same when self-rated health, emotional exhaustion and/or depression symptoms, one measure at a time or several simultaneously, were added as covariates at T1. Awakening Problems presents the model fit and model comparisons for models on demand, control, support and awakening problems. All models concerning awakening problems also provided a good or very good fit to the data. The only model including demands that differed significantly from the null model was the reciprocal model.

In contrast, all competing models on decision authority and support showed significantly better fit to the data than Model 0. For both decision authority and support, again Model 3, with reciprocal paths, demonstrated the best fit to the data by means of the χ 2 difference test. Neither the forward nor the reverse paths for demands reached statistical significance, whereas both paths were significant and negative in the case of decision authority and support (.) For decision authority the standardized coefficients were of equal magnitude, while there was a stronger negative association between T1 awakening problems and T2 support than between T1 support and T2 awakening problems.

All significant coefficients were in the expected direction. Factor loadings can be found in the. As for disturbed sleep, the results were not markedly affected by additional adjustment for health measures.

DISCUSSION The results indicated forward as well as reverse and reciprocal relationships between work characteristics and sleep problems 2 years later. Our finding of a close to significant positive association between demands and later sleep disturbances is in accordance with the recent study by de Lange et al., as well as with a study by Ota et al. Which showed a positive prospective relationship between job strain and insomnia. Contrary to De Lange et al., however, who found positive effects of both demands and control on sleep quality and fatigue, our results did not indicate a forward effect of job control, measured by decision authority, on sleep disturbances. We did, on the other hand, find a forward effect on awakening problems. Although the measures of fatigue by de Lange et al. And our measure of awakening problems have one item in common (not well rested at awakening), fatigue in their study otherwise seem to refer to daytime fatigue rather than non-restorative sleep.

Concerning support, Ota et al. Did not find a prospective relationship with insomnia among those who were not insomniacs at baseline. However, among those with insomnia at baseline, low support was associated with insomnia two years later.

No significant forward effect on sleep disturbances could be demonstrated in the present study either, although a tendency was observed, as well as a weak but significant direct effect of support on awakening problems. Interpret their results as indicating no association between support and onset of insomnia, but an association between support and persistence of insomnia. They suggest that sufficient support may facilitate recovery from insomnia and further discuss a vicious circle of stress and insomnia through hyperreactivity of the HPA and SAM systems.

However, the only study we have found that specifically investigated reverse and reciprocal associations between work stress and sleep problems did not find support for reverse or reciprocal links between demand or control and sleep quality/fatigue, using a 3-year time lag. Conversely, our present study, based on an approximately representative sample drawn from the working age population and with different measures of sleep problems/fatigue, indicates a reciprocal pattern for decision authority and awakening problems.

In our study, a 2-year instead of a 3-year time lag is used, which may explain the differences in results. In the present study, reverse and reciprocal patterns were also demonstrated for support, which was not investigated by De Lange et al.

The reverse paths related to support actually showed the strongest regression coefficients in the present study. This is in line with research showing that people with insomnia usually report more daytime problems, including problems with social functioning/social relationships., A prospective association between insomnia and low social support has also been shown among adolescents. Yet, to our knowledge a prospective association between sleep problems and social support at work has not been clearly demonstrated in previous research. Future studies will have to reveal whether our findings on reverse/reciprocal associations are spurious or due to a changed experience of the work environment. There is, however, some evidence of reverse or reciprocal relationships for related health problems like poor mental health, suggesting that poor mental health may affect the experience of work stress.

– A few studies on support prospectively link affective strains like irritation, depression, anger, and loneliness to decreased workplace support, as well as emotional exhaustion to social support from supervisors., No reverse causation in relation to emotional exhaustion, however, was demonstrated for support in a study using a 1-year interval or in another with a 2-year time lag, though the study samples were small. Different mechanisms explaining reverse effects of mental health on work have also been put forward. First, two types of changes or a combination of the two can occur as a result of poor health—either a change in the perception of the same work environment, or an actual change in the work environment. The authors further speculate that people may perceive their work in either a rosier or gloomier light depending on their health status. There may also be an upward selection mechanism into more favorable job situations or a drift mechanism, according to which people with poor mental health would drift to more unfavorable jobs. All but the drift mechanism were supported in an analysis of healthy and unhealthy job changers versus stayers.

Both perceptional and environmental changes could be hypothesized to be consequences of sleep problems. Poor sleep may be associated with mood changes, as well as decreased cognitive abilities, which could influence how the work environment is perceived. Difficulties with social relationships have also been demonstrated., Reduced productivity/efficiency and work ability are other probable consequences of sleep problems., Changes in mood, social functioning, or decreased work efficiency could in turn influence the behavior of superiors/colleagues. As a result, the employee may experience actual changes in support from superiors/colleagues, control at work, or in work demands, within the same job or through job transfer. Awakening problems in particular, which may by a stronger indicator of inadequate sleep/fatigue, could be suspected to be linked to a more negative perception of the work environment or changed behaviors by superiors/coworkers. It is also interesting that both social support and decision authority showed significant reciprocal relationships with awakening problems.

Dsa Wege Des Schwartz Pdf Editor

This may be an illustration of a negative spiral, with low decision authority and poor support causing awakening problems, which in turn cause reduced experience of decision authority and social support. The present study has a number of advantages compared with much of the previous literature on work characteristics and sleep problems. It is not restricted to specific segments of the labor market, but based on a large, approximately representative sample of the Swedish workforce. This contributes to good generalizability, and allows the examination of a wide range of scores on job stress and sleep problems. Even more importantly, it is longitudinal with repeat measures of both work stressors and sleep problems. By examining different types of plausible causal relationships, the study contributes to a more comprehensive understanding of causality between work stress and sleep problems.

There are, however, some methodological limitations. All measures are based on survey data. Thanks to the longitudinal design, some problems associated with using self-report data are avoided, such as influence by occasion factors (e.g., mood at the time of survey). However, other unmeasured third variables, such as negative affectivity and social desirability, may still affect our results.

Dsa Wege Des Schwartz Pdf Download

Health problems could be a common cause of work stress and sleep problems and create spurious associations. However, health problems at Time 1 did not seem to influence the results. As in most longitudinal studies, attrition may bias the results, possibly resulting in an underestimation of true relationships. No major bias due to drop-out was indicated in a comparison between the study sample and all SWES participants.

It is, however, unclear if non-response to the SWES and LFS could have any influence on results. The results show relatively low regression coefficients. This is to be expected, as the stability of the estimates, which was relatively high, was adjusted for by the inclusion of auto-regressions over time, in effect adjusting for the effects of previous work stress and sleep problems. A large part of the variability is thus already explained, and only changes are predicted. By allowing correlations also between measures at the same time point, cross-sectional relationships between work stress and sleep are additionally accounted for by the models. Moreover, the present modeling approach does not account for hypothesized interaction between demands, decision authority, and support and stress reactions. According to the strain and iso-strain hypotheses, stronger associations may be expected between job strain (a combination of high demands and low control), especially in combination with poor support (iso-strain) and sleep problems.

The fact that all variables are modeled as linear variables may also reduce the estimates of the associations, for instance if a certain degree of work stress is necessary for an effect on sleep. Another issue concerns whether the measures represent transient or enduring problems with stress and sleep. Cumulative exposures to strain and active work have been shown to have relevance for sleep problems, and an improvement in these working conditions does not necessarily results an immediate reduction in sleep problems.

Dsa

The causes and consequences of persistent problems may be different from those of temporary problems as indicated by Ota et al. Finally, the time lag in the present study could be discussed. A 2-year time lag has been found appropriate when studying the effect of social stressors on irritation and depressive symptoms. However, if the true causal lag time concerning sleep problems is shorter or longer than 2 years, the study is not optimally designed for detecting those particular associations. Most of the effects of transient stress on sleep are likely to be immediate, mediated by worrying thoughts.

The true time lag for forward and reverse effects, or for different types of reverse effects, may also differ, which could influence the results. Reverse effects explained by perceptional changes may have short time lag, whereas other changes may take longer.

CONCLUSIONS The findings of this cross-lagged analysis indicate a weak effect of demands on later sleep disturbances, an effect of sleep disturbances on later ratings on social support, and reciprocal relationship between awakening problems and work characteristics. In contrast to an earlier study on demands, control, sleep quality, and fatigue, this study suggests that there are reverse and reciprocal causal relationships, in addition to associations in the usually hypothesized direction, between work characteristics and sleep problems based on a 2-year time lag. Further studies should investigate these relationships with varying time lags and several waves of data. Figure S1: Basic LISREL model with standard solution estimates for the model including reciprocal paths between demands (D) and disturbed sleep (DS). Please observe that the time points are aligned from top to bottom of the figure. Variables ending with 1 refer to variables measured at time point 1 (2006) while variables ending with 2 refer to variables measured at time point 2 (2008).

A-e denotes the specific items included in the latent variables: Da, enough time; Db, working fast; Dc, working intensively; Dd, too much effort; De, conflicting demands; DSa, difficulty falling asleep; DSb, repeated awakenings; DSc, early awakening; DSd, disturbed sleep. Sex, Gender (women vs men); Age, Age; Marit, Marital status (married/cohabiting vs single); Educ, Education; Alcoh, Alcohol consumption; Jobch, Job change between 2006 and 2008. Figure S2: Basic LISREL model with standard solution estimates for the model including reciprocal paths between decision authority (DA) and disturbed sleep (DS). Please observe that the time points are aligned from top to bottom of the figure. Variables ending with 1 refer to variables measured at time point 1 (2006) while variables ending with 2 refer to variables measured at time point 2 (2008).

Dsa Wege Des Schwartz Pdf

A-d denotes the items included in the latent variables: DAa, choice in how you do your work; DAb, choice in what you do at work; DSa, difficulty falling asleep; DSb, repeated awakenings; DSc, early awakening; DSd, disturbed sleep. Sex, Gender (women vs men); Age, Age; Marit, Marital status (married/cohabiting vs single); Educ, Education; Alcoh, Alcohol consumption; Jobch, Job change between 2006 and 2008. Figure S3: Basic LISREL model with standard solution estimates for the model including reciprocal paths between social support (S) and disturbed sleep (DS).

Please observe that the time points are aligned from top to bottom of the figure. Variables ending with 1 refer to variables measured at time point 1 (2006) while variables ending with 2 refer to variables measured at time point 2 (2008). A-f denotes the items included in the latent variables: Sa, calm and pleasant atmosphere; Sb, good spirit of unity; Sc, colleagues are there for me; Sd, people understand a bad day; Se, get on well with my superiors; Sf, get on well with my colleagues; DSa, difficulty falling asleep; DSb, repeated awakenings; DSc, early awakening; DSd, disturbed sleep. Sex, Gender (women vs men); Age, Age; Marit, Marital status (married/cohabiting vs single); Educ, Education; Alcoh, Alcohol consumption; Jobch, Job change between 2006 and 2008. Figure S4: Basic LISREL model with standard solution estimates for the model including reciprocal paths between demands (D) and awakening problems (AP). Please observe that the time points are aligned from top to bottom of the figure. Variables ending with 1 refer to variables measured at time point 1 (2006) while variables ending with 2 refer to variables measured at time point 2 (2008).

A-e denotes the specific items included in the latent variables: Da, enough time; Db, working fast; Dc, working intensively; Dd, too much effort; De, conflicting demands; APa, difficulty awakening; APb, not well-rested; APc, exhausted at awakening. Sex, Gender (women vs men); Age, Age; Marit, Marital status (married/cohabiting vs single); Educ, Education; Alcoh, Alcohol consumption; Jobch, Job change between 2006 and 2008. Figure S5: Basic LISREL model with standard solution estimates for the model including reciprocal paths between decision authority (DA) and awakening problems (AP).

Dsa Wege Des Schwartz Pdf Converter

Please observe that the time points are aligned from top to bottom of the figure. Variables ending with 1 refer to variables measured at time point 1 (2006) while variables ending with 2 refer to variables measured at time point 2 (2008).

A-d denotes the items included in the latent variables: DAa, choice in how you do your work; DAb, choice in what you do at work; APa, difficulty awakening; APb, not well-rested; APc, exhausted at awakening. Sex, Gender (women vs men); Age, Age; Marit, Marital status (married/cohabiting vs single); Educ, Education; Alcoh, Alcohol consumption; Jobch, Job change between 2006 and 2008. Figure S6: Basic LISREL model with standard solution estimates for the model including reciprocal paths between social support (S) and awakening problems (AP). Please observe that the time points are aligned from top to bottom of the figure.

Variables ending with 1 refer to variables measured at time point 1 (2006) while variables ending with 2 refer to variables measured at time point 2 (2008). A-f denotes the items included in the latent variables: Sa, calm and pleasant atmosphere; Sb, good spirit of unity; Sc, colleagues are there for me; Sd, people understand a bad day; Se, get on well with my superiors; Sf, get on well with my colleagues; APa, difficulty awakening; APb, not well-rested; APc, exhausted at awakening. Sex, Gender (women vs men); Age, Age; Marit, Marital status (married/cohabiting vs single); Educ, Education; Alcoh, Alcohol consumption; Jobch, Job change between 2006 and 2008.